Aphasia

Evidence Reviewed as of before: 27-11-2018
Author(s)*: Tatiana Ogourtsova, PhD OT; Laura Chartrand; Amy Henderson PhD student; Sandra Bélisle-L’Anglais; Julie-France Hénault; Lyna Kaing; Caroline Lévesque; Sia Ching Tran; Véronique Vaillant; Adam Kagan BSc; Annabel McDermott BOccThy
Expert Reviewer: Elizabeth Rochon
Content consistency: Gabriel Plumier
Patient/Family Information Table of contents

Introduction

Aphasia is a language impairment that typically results from damage to the left hemisphere of the brain and consequently, is a common impairment after stroke. The resulting language dysfunctions are roughly classified as expressive (e.g. Broca’s aphasia) or receptive (e.g. Wernicke’s aphasia).

An individual with Broca’s aphasia has a partial or total inability to speak or produce spontaneous speech. They often have no difficulty understanding others, however, thoughts and intentions are difficult to express and may be non-fluent in nature. This form of aphasia can also affect written communication.

An individual with Wernicke’s aphasia has a partial or total inability to understand spoken and sometimes written language. Expression of language may be fluent in nature, but can be difficult for the listener to understand.

The symptoms of global aphasia are those of severe Broca’s aphasia and Wernicke’s aphasia combined. There is an almost total reduction of all aspects of spoken and written language, in expression as well as comprehension.

This module contains 34 studies, where 21 of them are high quality randomized clinical trials (RCTs), 11 are fair quality RCTs, and 2 are non-RCTs. The majority of interventions (18 studies) were studied among patients with chronic stroke (i.e. 6 months or more post-stroke).

Overall, we identified different interventions for post-stroke aphasia and those include: constraint-induced aphasia therapy; speech language therapy-unspecified, task-oriented aphasia therapy; semantic/phonological training, action-embedded therapy, intentional gestures and visual gestural cueing, supported communication, technology-assisted training (computer and devices), behavioral aphasia therapy, cognitive linguistic therapy, and narrative aphasia intervention.

Patient/Family Information

Authors: Tatiana Ogourtsova, PhD OT; Maxim Ben-Yakov, BSc PT; Nicol Korner-Bitensky, PhD OT

Contributors: Jennifer Sweeney, M.S., CCC-SLP, S-LP (C) Founder, BC Aphasia Centre; Aura Kagan PhD – Aphasia Institute

What is aphasia?

Aphasia is a problem of language and communication that makes it difficult to understand or produce speech. It may also affect reading and writing ability. Aphasia affects about 30% of people who have had a stroke.

Why do people get aphasia after a stroke?

Our brain has two main areas responsible for language. One is for understanding language through reading or listening. The other is for making language by writing or speaking. The type and severity of aphasia depends on the area of the brain that was damaged, and the amount of damage to the brain.

Are there different types of aphasia?

There are two types of aphasia:

  1. Receptive aphasia is when a person has difficulty understanding what he/she hears or reads.
  2. Expressive aphasia is when a person has difficulty expressing him/herself by writing or talking.

How can I recognize aphasia?

Some people have mild aphasia after a stroke and have difficulty expressing themselves. For example, they may have difficulty finding a word to describe an object. They may say the beginning of a sentence, such as:

“I would like to have a…”, and then pause, as though they are trying to think of the right word.

For instance, instead of asking:

“What do you want to eat?”

you can ask them:

“Do you want a banana?”

Some people with aphasia are not able to speak, but they are able to write down what they want to say. Others may not be able to write or speak after the stroke.

Some people with aphasia no longer understand what you are saying to them. They may understand when you speak in one language (for example, the first language they learned) but not in another language that they also knew before the stroke.

Some people with aphasia may be unable to read after the stroke. This often comes as a shock to the person when they pick up a magazine or newspaper and the letters and words on the page have no meaning to them.

To better understand how someone with aphasia is feeling, think about being alone in a foreign country where you do not understand the language. You cannot make your needs understood and when people speak to you, you don’t understand them. This can be very frustrating and isolating.

It is important to remember that someone with aphasia can retain many of the cognitive and social skills he/she had before the stroke. However, these skills may be hidden or masked by the language difficulties. Individuals with aphasia are often incorrectly treated as though they are less capable. This can affect their social life and participation in community activities, which in turn can impact on their self-esteem and quality of life.

Who diagnoses and treats aphasia?

Speech-language pathologists (SLP’s), also known as speech therapists, are trained to diagnose the different types of aphasia and treat individuals with aphasia.

Other members of the stroke team, including nurses, psychologists or neuro-psychologists, occupational therapists, social workers, doctors/neurologists, and physical therapists also understand and can help with the difficulties caused by aphasia. They will try to communicate with the person who has aphasia using strategies that are known to make communication easier.

The SLP will also explain to the stroke team the specific problems that the person with aphasia is having, and will suggest strategies to make communication easier.

Does Speech Language Therapy (SLT) work?

Yes, SLT works! A recent high-quality study found that SLT can improve different skills related to language and communication in individuals in the chronic phase of stroke recovery (more than 6 months after stroke).

Are there different types of SLT?

Numerous SLT approaches exist for aphasia. Your SLP will help choose a suitable program according to your needs. These might include:

  • Constraint-induced aphasia therapy: you practice having a conversation with your therapist or peers mostly without using gestures or other non-verbal communication (e.g. nodding your head).
  • Task-oriented aphasia therapy: you will train to follow spoken commands that are important to your everyday activities.
  • Semantic treatment: your therapist will ask you questions about words that are difficult for you to remember, and you will answer them. For example, if you have difficulty remembering the word ‘television’, the therapist will ask you questions such as: ‘where is the object located?’ [‘living room’]; or ‘what is it used for? [‘to watch movies’].
  • Action-embedded therapy: you perform actions to match a verbal request, the name of a word or something you visualise in your mind.
  • Intentional gestures: you move your left hand to do a task (e.g. reaching for a box, pressing a button), while naming different objects shown on pictures.
  • Supported communication: you join in group discussions or talk with a trained conversation partner using both verbal and non-verbal communication (i.e. words, gestures, writing notes, miming, role play, etc.).
  • Technology-assisted training: you use computer programs or other technological devices in your treatment sessions.
  • Behavioral aphasia therapy: your therapist will provide you with education, activities and tasks to improve your mood.
  • Cognitive-linguistic therapy: your therapist will work with you to practice saying words and sentences, based on logic/decision-making and organization of sounds in words.
  • Narrative aphasia intervention: you practice saying words and sentences while discussing a range of everyday topics (for example: shopping for an item).
  • Very early intervention: you engage in various aphasia therapies as early as possible after having a stroke and when medically stable.

You might have also heard about recent high-tech treatments. For example, computer programs for aphasia can be very helpful for some people after stroke. These computer programs include microphones and recording devices that allow people with aphasia to practice talking. The person can hear him/herself speak and can correct their mistakes. Computer programs can also help people re-learn to read after a stroke. Computer programs allow you to practice in your own home, in your own time. You can use computer programs in addition to speech language pathology to practice techniques and special skills that your SLP has taught you.

Your SLP may choose one or a combination of these different therapies when working with you, based on your individual needs.

What therapies work for aphasia?

Research has tested the effect of aphasia therapies at different stages of stroke recovery. High and fair quality research studies show that the following interventions are useful in improving language and/or communication skills:

In the acute stage of stroke recovery
(up to 1 month after stroke):

  • constraint-induced aphasia therapy
  • very early intervention

In the subacute stage of stroke recovery
(1-6 months after stroke):

  • phonological training
  • semantic training

In the chronic stage of stroke recovery
(more than 6 months after stroke):

  • action-embedded therapy
  • speech language therapy-unspecified
  • supported communication
  • technology-assisted training

Studies done with patients who were across the recovery continuum (i.e. acute, subacute and/or chronic):

  • cognitive-behavioral aphasia therapy
  • cognitive-linguistic therapy
  • speech language therapy-unspecified

What can I expect from aphasia therapies?

You can expect to work individually or in small groups with help from your SLP. The SLP might use different approaches such as cards with pictures, electronic devices, stories, video and audio supports, reading and writing material. Your SLP might also provide you with exercises and activities to do in your room or at home, with or without a caregiver/conversational partner.

Do I need to do SLT for a long time?

Research suggests that the timing and intensity of treatment are very important. After a stroke, patients often spend a short time in hospital or a rehabilitation clinic where they receive SLT. They may not continue with SLT when they return home from hospital. However, it is very important to continue with SLT for aphasia.

Research shows that both one-to-one and group therapies are effective for aphasia. The goals of each type of therapy differ. One-to-one therapy may be best for improving speech, whereas group therapy helps with conversation and social skills. So, if you or your family member has aphasia, consider participating in both one-to-one and group therapy if available.

Are there any side effects?

You might experience some side effects from aphasia therapies such as mental fatigue and/or frustration. It is important for your therapist to be aware of any side effects that you might be experiencing so that your treatment regime can be adjusted to better fit your abilities and need

How does aphasia affect my recovery from stroke?

Stroke recovery requires a lot of patience and perseverance from the person who had a stroke and their family/caregivers. If you or your loved one is experiencing aphasia after stroke, the process of recovery might be more frustrating and challenging. It is important to continue with therapies, even if aphasia makes it challenging.

Will my aphasia get better?

Yes. There is evidence that language and communication skills can improve after stroke with different aphasia therapies.

There is also evidence that “higher intensity” SLT (more sessions and/or for a longer duration) can result in greater and/or faster gains.

It is important to remember that the rate at which someone improves, and whether they make a complete or a partial recovery, can vary from one person to another and depends on many individual factors

How long does it take to recover?

The recovery from aphasia can take time. However, there is evidence that speech-language therapy works even in patients who have had the stroke years ago.

My family member has aphasia. How can I help?

Aphasia is stressful for the whole family. Research shows that education sessions are beneficial for family members and friends, as it can increase awareness and knowledge about aphasia. Below are some tips on communicating with people with aphasia.

To help the person with aphasia understand what you are saying:

  • Look at the person when you are speaking to them.
  • Use gestures and facial expressions to help them understand what you are saying.
  • Write down key words. Use a thick black marker and print letters.
  • Draw a picture or symbol of what you are trying to say.
  • Use objects to help get your message across (e.g. if asking the person if they want a drink, hold up a cup).
  • Use yes/no questions.
  • Acknowledge that the person with aphasia is a competent, knowledgeable person who can make decisions, and that they usually know what they want to say, but cannot say it.
  • Speak in a tone of voice appropriate for an adult. Do not sound condescending, or like you are speaking to a child.
  • Communicate one idea at a time. For example, instead of saying:“I will help you get up, showered and dressed and then I am going to take you to the dining room for breakfast but first you have to take your pill.”You might try this:
    • “Here is your pill” (pause and give pill).
    • “First I will help you take a shower” (pause).
    • “Then I will help you get dressed” (pause).
    • “Then I will take you for breakfast.”

The following scenario between a care provider and a patient helps to illustrate the above strategies:

Conversation Strategies used by careprovider
“Mrs. Jones, I want to tell you about an event we have coming up. I think you will be interested.” Comes prepared with a calendar, scrap paper and a black marker.
“This is today.” Points to the date on the calendar.
“On Tuesday…” Points to the date on the calendar and then writes it down.
“…the John Higgins Band is coming to play.” Writes down “John Higgins Band”.
“They are coming here.” Uses hand gestures to indicate the location.
“Do you want to go?” Writes down “Do you want to go?” and points to the resident. Writes down:

Yes / No / I don’t know

and waits for the patient to point to the response.

To help the person with aphasia to get their message across:

  • Encourage the person with aphasia to write down a word or draw if they can.
  • Encourage the person with aphasia to point to something.
  • Ask questions to identify the general topic first and then move onto understanding the details. For example:
    • “Are you talking about your family?”
    • “Are you talking about your daughter?”
    • “Are you wondering if she is coming to visit today?”
  • Ask yes/no questions.
  • Use a written yes/no if needed.
  • If you do not have the time to communicate, explain this and give a time when you will return to finish your conversation. Make sure you do return.
  • Encourage the person with aphasia to use some of the words you have written down to communicate. For example:
    • “I have heard you are a big fan of music?”
    • “What kind of music do you like?”
    • “Opera? … Classical? … Blues?”

Where can I find more information about aphasia?

For further detail about aphasia, please visit these links:

Clinician Information

Note: When reviewing the findings, it is important to note that they are always made according to randomized clinical trial (RCT) criteria – specifically as compared to a control group. To clarify, if a treatment is “effective” it implies that it is more effective than the control treatment to which it was compared. Non-randomized studies are no longer included when there is sufficient research to indicate strong evidence (level 1a) for an outcome.

This module contains 34 studies, where 21 of them are high quality randomized clinical trials (RCTs), 11 are fair quality RCTs, and two are non-RCTs. The majority of interventions (18 studies) were studied among patients with chronic stroke (i.e. 6 months or more post-stroke).

Overall, we identified different interventions for post-stroke aphasia and those include: constraint-induced aphasia therapy; speech language therapy-unspecified, task-oriented aphasia therapy; semantic / phonological training, action-embedded therapy, intentional gestures and visual gestural cueing, supported communication, technology-assisted training (computer and devices), behavioral aphasia therapy, cognitive linguistic therapy, and narrative aphasia intervention.

Click here to access resources from the Aphasia Institute (Ontario, Canada).

Results Table

View results table

Outcomes

Acute phase - Constraint-induced aphasia therapy

Aphasia
Not Effective
1A

Two high quality RCT (Ciccone et al., 2016; Woldag et al., 2017) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on aphasia severity in patients with acute stroke.

The first high quality RCT (Ciccone et al., 2016) randomized patients to receive CIAT or individual conventional speech language therapy. Aphasia was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Aphasia Quotient at post-treatment (4-5 weeks) and follow-up (12 and 26 weeks). No between-group differences were found at any time point.

The second high quality RCT (Woldag et al., 2017) randomized patients to receive CIAT, high-intensity conventional communication group language therapy or low-intensity individual/group speech language therapy. Aphasia was measured by the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT: 50-item Token test, Repetition, Written language, Naming, Comprehension, Profile level scores) at post-treatment (2 weeks). No between-group differences were found.

Conclusion: There is strong evidence (Level 1a) from two high quality RCTs that CIAT is not more effective than comparison interventions (individual conventional speech language therapy; high-intensity conventional communication group language therapy; low-intensity individual/group speech language therapy) in improving aphasia in patients with acute stroke.

Communication accuracy/efficiency
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Ciccone et al., 2016) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on communication accuracy/efficiency in patients with acute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive CIAT or individual conventional speech language therapy. Communication accuracy/efficiency was measured by a Discourse Analysis score (percent correct information units produced per minute) at post-treatment (4-5 weeks) and follow-up (12 and 26 weeks). No between-group differences were found at any time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that CIAT is not more effective than a comparison intervention (individual conventional speech language therapy) in improving communication accuracy/efficiency in patients with acute stroke.

Communication quantity/quality
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Woldag et al., 2017) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on communication quantity/quality in patients with acute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive CIAT, high-intensity conventional communication group language therapy or low-intensity individual/group speech language therapy. Communication quantity/quality was measured by the Communicative Activity Log (CAL – Quality, Quantity scores) at post-treatment (2 weeks). A significant between-group difference was found on one measure (CAL – Quality score), favoring CIAT vs. high-intensity conventional communication group language therapy. No other significant between-group differences were found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that CIAT is more effective than a comparison intervention (high-intensity conventional communication group language therapy) in improving communication quality in patients with acute stroke.
Note: CIAT was not more effective than low-intensity individual/group speech language therapy in improving communication quality; CIAT was not more effective than comparison interventions (high-intensity conventional communication group language therapy) in improving communications quantity.

Quality of life
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Ciccone et al., 2016) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on quality of life in patients with acute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive CIAT or individual conventional speech language therapy. Quality of life was measured by the Stroke and Aphasia Quality of Life Scale at post-treatment (4-5 weeks) and follow-up (12 and 26 weeks). No between-group difference was found at any time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that CIAT is not more effective than a comparison intervention (individual conventional speech language therapy) in improving quality of life in patients with acute stroke.

Acute phase - Speech language therapy-unspecified

Aphasia
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Laska et al., 2011) and one fair quality RCT (Mattioli et al., 2014) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on aphasia in patients with acute stroke.

The high quality RCT (Laska et al., 2011) randomized patients to receive speech language therapy or no treatment. Aphasia was measured by the Norsk Grunntest for Afasi (NGA: Aphasia coefficient) at post-treatment (21 days) and at follow-up (6 months). No significant between group difference was found at either time point.

The fair quality RCT (Mattioli et al., 2014) randomized patients to receive speech language therapy or no treatment. Aphasia was measured by the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT: Repetition, Naming, Written language, Oral comprehension, Written comprehension, 50-item Token Test, Spontaneous language subtests) at post-treatment (2 weeks) and at follow-up (6 months). Significant between-group differences were found on only two subtests (AAT: Naming, Written language) at post-treatment, favoring speech language therapy vs. no treatment. Improvements were maintained at follow-up.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT and one fair quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving aphasia in patients with acute stroke.

Carer wellbeing
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Bowen et al., 2012) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on the wellbeing of carers of patients with acute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive speech language therapy or social contact with an untrained conversation partner for 16 weeks. Carer wellbeing was measured by the Carers of Older People in Europe Index (COPE: Negative/Positive impact, Quality of support) at follow-up (6 months post-stroke). No significant between-group differences were found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (social contact with an untrained conversation partner) in improving the wellbeing of carers of patients with acute stroke.

Functional communication ability
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Bowen et al., 2012) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on functional communication ability in patients with acute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive speech language therapy or social contact with an untrained conversation partner for 16 weeks. Functional communication ability was measured by the Therapy Outcome Measure (TOM: Communicative Activity Scale) at follow-up (6 months post-stroke). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (social contact with an untrained conversation partner) in improving functional communication ability in patients with acute stroke.

Self-perception of communication effectiveness
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Bowen et al., 2012) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on self-perception of communication effectiveness in patients with acute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive speech language therapy or social contact with an untrained conversation partner for 16 weeks. Patients’ and carers’ evaluation of patients’ communication effectiveness was measured by the Communication Outcomes After Stroke (COAST) and Carer COAST (respectively) at follow-up (6 months post-stroke). No significant between-group differences were found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (social contact with an untrained conversation partner) in improving patients’ and carers’ perception of communication effectiveness in patients with acute stroke.

Verbal communication
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Laska et al., 2011) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on verbal communication in patients with acute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to received speech language therapy or no treatment. Verbal communication skills were measured by the Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test at post-treatment (21 days) and at follow-up (6 months). No significant between group difference was found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving verbal communication in patients with acute stroke.

Acute phase - Very early intervention

Aphasia
Effective
2B

One non-randomized design study (Godecke et al., 2014) investigated the effect of very early intervention on aphasia in patients with acute stroke. This non-randomized design study allocated patients to receive a multimodal intervention or usual care (sample drawn from Godecke et al., 2012 cohort). Aphasia was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Aphasia Quotient (percentage maximum potential recovery) at post-treatment (5 weeks) and follow-up (26 weeks post-stroke). Significant between-group differences were found at post-treatment, favoring very early intervention vs. usual care. Improvements were maintained at follow-up.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2b) from one non-randomized design study that very early intervention is more effective than a comparison intervention (usual care) in improving aphasia in patients with acute stroke.

Communication accuracy/efficiency
Effective
2B

One non-randomized design study (Godecke et al., 2014) investigated the effect of very early intervention on communication accuracy/efficiency in patients with acute stroke. This non-randomized design study allocated patients to receive a multimodal intervention or usual care (sample drawn from Godecke et al., 2012 cohort). Communication accuracy/efficiency was measured by a Discourse Analysis score at post-treatment (5 weeks) and follow-up (26 weeks post-stroke). A significant between-group difference was found at post-treatment, favoring very early intervention vs. usual care. Improvements were not maintained at follow-up.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2b) from one non-randomized design study that very early intervention is more effective than a comparison intervention (usual care) in improving communication accuracy/efficiency in patients with acute stroke.

Subacute phase - Constraint-induced aphasia therapy

Aphasia
Non Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Sickert et al., 2014) investigated the effect of modified Constraint Induced Aphasia Therapy (mCIAT) on aphasia in patients with subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive mCIAT or conventional aphasia therapy. Aphasia was measured by the Aachener Aphasia Test (Spontaneous speech, Token Test, Repetition, Written language, Naming, Comprehension) at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (8 weeks, 1 year). No significant between-group differences were found at any time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that mCIAT is not more effective that a comparison intervention (conventional therapy) in improving aphasia in patients with subacute stroke.

Communication quantity/quality
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Sickert et al., 2014) investigated the effect of modified Constraint Induced Aphasia Therapy (mCIAT) on communication quantity/quality in patients with subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive mCIAT or conventional aphasia therapy. Communication quantity/quality was measured by the Communicative Activity Log (CAL – Quality, Quantity scores: patient/relative scores) at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (8 weeks, 1 year). No significant between-group differences were found at any time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that mCIAT is not more effective that a comparison intervention (conventional therapy) in improving communication quantity/quality in patients with subacute stroke.

Subacute phase - Semantic vs. phonological treatment approaches

Language processing
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Doesborgh et al., 2004a) compared the effect of semantic vs. phonological intervention approaches on language processingin patients with subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive semantic (interpretation-focused) or phonological (sound structure focused) treatment approaches. Language processing skills were measured by the Semantic Association Test [semantic measure] and the Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA: synonym judgement [semantic measure], nonword repetition, auditory lexical decision [phonological measures]) at post-treatment (10-12 months). A significant between-group difference was found on one phonological measure (PALPA: auditory lexical decision), favoring phonological vs. semantic treatment approach.
Note: A statistically-significant within-group improvement in one semantic measure (Semantic Association Test) was seen following semantic treatment, and an improvement in both phonological measures was seen following phonological treatment.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that a phonological treatment approach is more effective than a semantic treatment approach in improving phonological language processing skills in patients with subacute stroke.

Verbal communication
Not Efective
1B

One high quality RCT (Doesborgh et al., 2004a) compared the effect of semantic vs. phonological intervention approaches on verbal communication in patients with subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive semantic (interpretation-focused) or phonological (sound structure focused) treatment approaches. Verbal communication skills were measured by the Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday Language Test (ANELT-A: final score, mean improvement) at post-treatment (10-12 months). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that a semantic treatment approach is not more effective than a phonological treatment approach in improving verbal communication in patients with subacute stroke.

Subacute phase - Speech language therapy-unspecified

Carers' perception of patient's language
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Lincoln et al., 1984) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on carers’ perception of language in patients with subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive speech language therapy or no treatment. Carer’s perception of patient’s expressive and receptive language was measured by the Speech Questionnaire at mid-treatment (12 weeks) and post-treatment (24 weeks). No significant between-group difference was found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving carers’ perception of language of patients with subacute stroke.

Communication
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Lincoln et al., 1984) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on communication in patients with subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive speech language therapy or no treatment. Communication skills were measured by the Porch Index of Communicative Ability and the Functional Communication Profile at mid-treatment (12 weeks) and post-treatment (24 weeks). No significant between-group differences were found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving communication skills in patients with subacute stroke.

Subacute phase - Task-oriented aphasia therapy

Communication
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Hartman & Laundau, 1987) investigated the effect of task-oriented aphasia therapy in improving communication in patients with subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive task-oriented aphasia therapy or nondirective counseling and conversation. Communication skills were measured by the Porch Index of Communicative Ability at post-treatment (6 months) and at follow-up (10 months post-stroke). No significant between-group difference was found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that task-oriented aphasia therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (nondirective counseling and conversation) in improving communication skills in patients with subacute stroke.

Chronic phase - Action-embedded therapy

Aphasia
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Stahl et al., 2016) investigated the effect of action-embedded therapy on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality cross-over design RCT randomized patients to receive intensive language action therapy (ILAT) or naming therapy for 6 days, then the other intervention for a further 6 days; participants received no therapy for 6 days between intervention periods. Language was measured by the Aachen Aphasia Test (AAT: mean score, production score) at post-treatment (6 days, 18 days). Significant between-group differences were found on both measures at both post-treatment time points, favoring ILAT vs. naming therapy.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive language action therapy is more effective than a comparison intervention (naming therapy) in improving aphasia in patients with chronic stroke.

Word retrieval response time
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Drummond & Rentschler, 1981) investigated the effect of action-embedded therapy on word retrieval response time in patients with chronic stroke. This fair quality RCT randomized patients to receive visual-gestural cueing training or no gesture cueing; both groups received auditory-verbal cueing. Word retrieval response time was measured by the response time to 10 AMERIND Gestural Code nouns and 10 non-AMERIND Gestural Code nouns at post-treatment (2 weeks). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that visual-gestural cueing training is not more effective than a comparison intervention (no gesture cueing) in improving word retrieval response time in patients with chronic stroke.

Chronic phase - Communication partner program

Aphasia
Not Effective
2B

One non-randomized study (Lyon et al., 1997) investigated the effect of a communication partner program on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke. This non-randomized study assigned patients and their carers to a communication partner training program with a community volunteer. Aphasia was measured by the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE: overall mean percentile) at post-treatment (20 weeks). No significant improvements from pre- to post-treatment were found.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2b) from one non-randomized study that a communication partner training program with a community volunteer is not effective in improving aphasia in patients with chronic stroke.

Communication
Not Effective
2B

One non-randomized study (Lyon et al., 1997) investigated the effect of a communication partner program on communication in patients with chronic stroke. This non-randomized study assigned patients and their carers to a communication partner training program with a community volunteer. Communication skills were measured by the Communication Abilities in Daily Living (CADL) and the non-standardized Communication Readiness and Use Index (CRUI: patient, carer, communication partner scores) at post-treatment (20 weeks). Significant improvements from pre- to post-treatment were found on the CRUI.
Note: an improvement in communication readiness and use (CRUI) was also seen among patient triads whose treatment was deferred.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2b) from one non-randomized study that a communication partner training program with a community volunteer is not effective in improving communication in patients with chronic stroke.
Note: However, the program was effective in improving patients, carers’ and communication partners’ reports of communication in patients with chronic stroke.

Participation in conversation
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Kagan et al., 2001) investigated the effect of a communication partner program on participation in conversation in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to interact with a conversation partner trained in the “Supported Conversation for Adults with Aphasia” program, or with an untrained volunteer. Participation in conversation was measured by the Measure of Participation in Conversation for Adults with Aphasia (MPCA: Interaction, Transaction scores) at post-treatment (1-day workshop). Significant between-group differences were found on both scores, favoring the “Supported Conversation for Adults with Aphasia” program vs. social interactions with an untrained volunteer.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that a communication partner program is more effective than a comparison intervention (social interactions with an untrained volunteer) in improving participation in conversation in patients with chronic stroke.

Well-being
Not Effective
2B

One non-randomized study (Lyon et al., 1997) investigated the effect of a communication partner program on well-being in patients with chronic stroke. This non-randomized study assigned patients and their carers to a communication partner training program with a community volunteer. Well-being was measured by the Affect Balance Scale (ABS) and the non-standardized Psychosocial Wellbeing Index (PWI: patient, carer, communication partner scores) at post-treatment (20 weeks). A significant improvement from pre- to post-treatment was found on the PWI scores.
Note: an improvement in wellbeing (PWI) was also seen among patient triads whose treatment was deferred.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2b) from one non-randomized study that a communication partner training program with a community volunteer is not effective in improving well-being in patients with chronic stroke.
Note: However, the program was effective in improving patients’, carers’ and communication partners’ reported perception of well-being in patients with chronic stroke.

Chronic phase - Constraint-induced aphasia therapy

Aphasia
Not Effective
1b

One high quality RCT (Kurland et al., 2016) and one fair quality RCT (Maher et al., 2006) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke.

The high quality RCT (Kurland et al., 2016) randomized patients to receive interactive language action therapy – constrained (ILAT- constrained) or a modified version of Promoting Aphasic Communicative Effectiveness – unconstrained (mPACE -unconstrained). Aphasia was measured by the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE: Aphasia severity, Auditory comprehension, Sentence repetition, Cookie Theft Description task – changes in content units) at post-treatment (2 weeks). No significant between-group difference was found.

The fair quality RCT (Maher et al., 2006) assigned patients to receive CIAT or mPACE. Aphasia was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Aphasia Quotient at post-treatment (2 weeks). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT and one fair quality RCT that constraint-induced aphasia therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (Promoting Aphasic Communicative Effectiveness) in improving aphasia in patients with chronic stroke.

Communication
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Kurland et al., 2016) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on communication in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive interactive language action therapy – constrained (ILAT-constrained) or a modified version of Promoting Aphasic Communicative Effectiveness – unconstrained (mPACE-unconstrained). Communication skills were measured by the Porch Index of Communicative Abilities at post-treatment (2 weeks). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that constraint-induced aphasia therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (modified version of Promoting Aphasic Communicative Effectiveness – unconstrained) in improving communication skills in patients with chronic stroke.

Naming
Not Effective
1a

Two high quality RCTs (Szaflarski et al., 2015; Kurland et al., 2016) and two fair quality RCTs (Maher et al., 2006; Nenert et al., 2017) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on naming in patients with chronic stroke.

The first high-quality RCT (Szaflarski et al., 2015) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment for 10 consecutive weekdays. Naming skills were measured by the Boston Naming Test (BNT) at follow-up (1 week and 3 months post-treatment). No significant between-group difference was found.

The second high quality RCT (Kurland et al., 2016) randomized patients to receive interactive language action therapy – constrained (ILAT-constrained) or a modified version of Promoting Aphasic Communicative Effectiveness – unconstrained (mPACE-unconstrained). Naming skills were measured by the Object and Action Naming Battery (objects and verbs/trained and untrained items) and BNT at post-treatment (2 weeks). No significant between-group differences were found.

The first fair quality RCT (Maher et al., 2006) assigned patients to receive CIAT or mPACE. Naming skills were measured by the BNT and Action Naming Test at post-treatment (2 weeks). No significant between-group differences were found.

The second fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment. Naming skills were measured by the BNT at post-treatment (10 days). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is strong evidence (Level 1a) from two high quality RCTs and two fair quality RCTs that constraint-induced aphasia therapy is not more effective than comparison interventions (no treatment, Promoting Aphasic Communicative Effectiveness) in improving naming skills in patients with chronic stroke.

Noun recall
Not Effective
2a

One fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on noun recall in patients with chronic stroke. This fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment. Noun recall was measured by a noun recall task at post-treatment (10 days). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that constraint-induced aphasia therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving noun recall in patients with chronic stroke.

Receptive language
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Szaflarski et al., 2015) and one fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on receptive language in patients with chronic stroke.

The high quality RCT (Szaflarski et al., 2015) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment for 10 consecutive weekdays. Receptive language skills were measured by the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE: Complex ideation subtest) and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III (PPVT-III) at follow-up (1 week and 3 months post-treatment). No significant between-group differences were found.

The fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment. Receptive language skills were measured by the PPVT-IV at post-treatment (10 days). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT and one fair quality RCT that constraint-induced aphasia therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving receptive language skills in patients with chronic stroke.

Subjective communication
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Szaflarski et al., 2015) and one fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on subjective communication in patients with chronic stroke.

The high quality RCT (Szaflarski et al., 2015) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment for 10 consecutive weekdays. Subjective communication skills (fluency, frequency of use, frequency of misunderstandings) were measured by the Mini-Communicative Activities Log (Mini-CAL) at follow-up (1 week and 3 months post-treatment). A significant between-group difference was found at follow-up (3 months post-treatment), favoring CIAT vs. no treatment.

The fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment. Subjective communication skills were measured by the Mini-CAL at post-treatment (10 days). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT and one fair quality RCT that constraint-induced aphasia therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving subjective communication skills in patients with chronic stroke.
Note: However, CIAT was more effective than no treatment in the long term.

Verbal function/fluency
Not Effective
1b

One high quality RCT (Szaflarski et al., 2015) and one fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) investigated the effect of constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT) on verbal function and fluency in patients with chronic stroke.

The high quality RCT (Szaflarski et al., 2015) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment for 10 consecutive weekdays. Verbal function/fluency was measured by the Semantic Fluency Test (SFT) and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) at follow-up (1 week and 3 months post-treatment). No significant between-group difference was found.

The fair quality RCT (Nenert et al., 2017) randomized patients to receive CIAT or no treatment. Verbal function/fluency was measured by the SFT and the COWAT (raw score) at post-treatment (10 days). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT and one fair quality RCT that constraint-induced aphasia therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving verbal function/fluency.

Chronic phase - Group communication

Aphasia
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Elman & Burnstein-Ellis, 1999) investigated the effect of group communication on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive group communication treatment or delayed group communication treatment (no treatment). Aphasia was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery-Aphasia Quotient (WAB-AQ) at post-treatment (4 months) and follow-up (5 months). A significant between-group difference was found at post-treatment, favoring group communication treatment vs. no treatment. This between-group difference was not maintained at follow-up.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that group communication is more effective than a comparison intervention (no treatment) in improving aphasia in patients with chronic stroke.

Communication
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Elman & Burnstein-Ellis, 1999) investigated the effect of group communication on functional communication in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive group communication treatment or delayed group communication treatment (no treatment). Communication skills were measured by the Communicative Abilities in Daily Living Test (CADL) and the Shortened Porch Index of Communicative Ability (SPICA) at post-treatment (4 months) and follow-up (5 months). A significant between-group difference was found on one measure (CADL), favoring group communication treatment vs. no treatment. This between-group difference was not maintained at follow-up.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that group communication is more effective than a comparison intervention (no treatment) in improving communication skills in patients with chronic stroke.

Chronic phase - Intentional gestures

Aphasia
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Altmann et al., 2014) investigated the effect of intentional gestures on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke. This fair quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive anomia treatment using intentional left-hand gestures or intensive anomia treatment without gestures. Aphasia was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Aphasia Quotient at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). No significant between-group difference was found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that intentional gestures is not more effective than comparison interventions without gestures (intensive anomia treatment) in improving aphasia in patients with chronic stroke

Discourse quantity/quality
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Altmann et al., 2014) investigated the effect of intentional gestures on discourse quality/quantity in patients with chronic stroke. This fair quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive anomia treatment using intentional left-hand gestures or intensive anomia treatment without gestures. Discourse quantity/quality was measured according to Discourse Quantity (Utterances, Words, Verbs, Nouns) and Discourse Quality (Grammatical, Correct information units, Propositions, Utterances with new information) at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). A significant between-group difference was found on only one measure (Discourse Quantity: Words) at follow-up, favoring intentional left-hand gestures + intensive anomia treatment vs. intensive anomia treatment alone.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that intentional gestures is not more effective than comparison interventions without gestures (intensive anomia treatment) in improving discourse quantity/quality in patients with chronic stroke.

Naming and retrieval
Not Effective
2A

Two fair quality RCTs (Altmann et al., 2014; Benjamin et al., 2014) investigated the effect of intentional gestures on naming and retrieval skills in patients with chronic stroke.

The first fair quality RCT (Altmann et al., 2014) randomized patients to receive intensive anomia treatment using intentional left-hand gestures or intensive anomia treatment without gestures. Naming and retrieval skills were measured by the Picture Naming probes, Category-Generation probes, and the Boston Naming Test at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). No significant between-group differences were found at either time point.

The second fair quality RCT (Benjamin et al., 2014) randomized patients to receive picture naming and category-member generation training using intentional left-hand gestures or picture naming and category-member generation training without gestures. Naming and retrieval skills were measured by the Picture Naming and Category-Generation probes at post-treatment (6 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). No significant between-group differences were found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from two fair quality RCTs that intentional gestures is not more effective than comparison interventions without gestures (intensive anomia treatment, picture naming and category-member generation) in improving naming and retrieval skills in patients with chronic stroke.

Chronic phase - Speech language therapy-unspecified

Aphasia
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Breitenstein et al., 2017) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive speech language therapy or no treatment. Aphasia was measured by the Sprachsystematisches APhasieScreening (SAPS: Total, Phonology, Lexicon, Syntax, Language comprehension, Language production scores) at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (6 months). Significant between-group differences in change scores from baseline to post-treatment were found (SAPS: Total, Lexicon, Syntax, Language comprehension, Language production). A significant difference was found on one measure at follow-up (SAPS: Phonology).

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive speech language therapy is more effective than no treatment in improving aphasia in patients with chronic stroke.

Functional communication
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Breitenstein et al., 2017) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on functional communication in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive speech language therapy for 3 weeks or no treatment. Functional communication was measured by the Communication Effectiveness Index at follow-up (6 months). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving long-term functional communication in patients with chronic stroke.

Nonverbal learning
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Breitenstein et al., 2017) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on nonverbal learning in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive speech language therapy or no treatment. Nonverbal learning was measured by the Nonverbal Learning Test at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (6 months). No significant between-group difference was found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving nonverbal learning in patients with chronic stroke.

Quality of life
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Breitenstein et al., 2017) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on quality of life in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive speech language therapy or no treatment. Quality of life was measured by the Stroke and Aphasia Quality of Life Scale-39 (SAQoL-39 – Total, Physical, Communication, Psychosocial, Energy scores) at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (6 months). A significant between-group difference in change scores from baseline to post-treatment was found on only one measure (SAQoL-39 – Total score).

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive speech language therapy is more effective than no treatment in improving quality of life in patients with chronic stroke.

Stroke severity
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Breitenstein et al., 2017) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on stroke severity in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive speech language therapy or no treatment. Stroke severity was measured by the modified Rankin Scale at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (6 months). No significant between-group difference was found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in reducing stroke severity in patients with chronic stroke.

Verbal communication
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Breitenstein et al., 2017) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on verbal communication in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive speech language therapy or no treatment. Verbal communication skills were measured by the Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday Language Test (ANELT: A-scale, B-scale) at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (6 months). A significant between-group difference in change scores from baseline to post-treatment was found (ANALT: A-scale).

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive speech language therapy is more effective than no treatment in improving verbal communication skills in patients with chronic stroke.

Visual attention and executive function
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Breitenstein et al., 2017) investigated the effect of speech language therapy on visual attention and executive function in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive speech language therapy or no treatment. Visual attention and executive function were measured by the Trail Making Test A and B at post-treatment (3 weeks) and follow-up (6 months). No significant between-group differences were found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive speech language therapy is not more effective than no treatment in improving visual attention and executive function in patients with chronic stroke.

Chronic phase - Technology-assisted training

Aphasia
Conflicting
4

Two fair quality RCTs (Katz & Wertz, 1992; Katz & Wertz, 1997) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke.

The first fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1992) randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation or no treatment. Aphasia was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Aphasia Quotient (WAB-AQ) at post-treatment (6 months). No significant between-group difference was found.

The second fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1997) randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation, or no treatment. Aphasia was measured by the WAB-AQ at post-treatment (6 months). Comparison of computer reading vs. non-language computer stimulation revealed a significant difference favoring computer reading. Comparison of computer reading vs. no treatment revealed a significant difference favouring computer reading.
Note: Comparison of non-language computer stimulation vs. no treatment revealed no significant between-group difference.

Conclusion: There is conflicting evidence (Level 4) from two fair quality RCTs regarding the effect of technology-assisted training on aphasia in patients with chronic stroke. While one fair quality RCT found that computer reading was not more effective than comparison interventions (non-language computer stimulation, no treatment); another fair quality RCT found that computer reading was more effective than comparison interventions (non-language computer stimulation, no treatment) in improving aphasia in patients with stroke.
Note: The larger sample size in the second study might explain the found differences that were not detected in the first study.

Cognition
Not Effective
1b

One high quality RCT (Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on cognition in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality cross-over design RCT randomized patients to receive intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device or nonlinguistic cognitive training for 4 weeks, before crossing over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks. Cognition was measured at post-treatment (4 weeks, 8 weeks) and follow-up (3 months) and included: auditory memory, measured by the Wechesler Memory Scale (auditory digit span); visual memory, measured by the Corsi Block Tapping Test; attention, measured by the Test of Attentional Performance (visual scanning subtest); and pattern recognition, measured by the Thurstone’s Primary Mental Ability Test (pattern recognition from subtest 10). No significant between-group differences were found at any time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device is not more effective than a comparison intervention (nonlinguistic cognitive training) in improving cognition in patients with chronic stroke.

Communication
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) and two fair quality RCTs (Katz & Wertz, 1992; Katz & Wertz, 1997) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on communication in patients with chronic stroke.

The high quality cross-over design RCT (Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) randomized patients to receive intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device or nonlinguistic cognitive training for 4 weeks, before crossing over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks. Communication skills were measured by a non-standardized dialogue test (linguistic score – trained/untrained items; communitive score – trained/untrained items), at post-treatment (4 weeks, 8 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). There were no significant between-group differences at either post-treatment timepoint. Significant between-group differences were found (communicative score – trained/untrained items) at follow-up, favoring the group that received nonlinguistic cognitive training followed by intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device.

The first fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1992) randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation or no treatment. Communication skills were measured by the Porch Index of Communicative Ability (PICA: Overall score, Reading, Writing, Verbal modalities) at post-treatment (6 months). Comparison of computer reading vs. non-language computer stimulation showed significant between-group differences (PICA: Overall, Writing, Verbal scores), favouring computer reading. Comparison of computer reading vs. no treatment revealed significant differences (PICA: Overall, Verbal scores), favouring computer reading.
Note: Comparison of non-language computer stimulation vs. no treatment revealed no significant differences.

The second fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1997) randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation, or no treatment. Communication skills were measured by the PICA (Overall score, Auditory, Verbal, Pantomime, Visual, Reading, Copying, Writing scores) at post-treatment (6 months). Comparison of computer reading vs. non-language computer stimulation revealed significant differences (PICA: Overall, Verbal scores), favoring computer reading. Comparison of computer reading vs. no treatment revealed significant differences (PICA: Overall, Verbal, Pantomime scores), favouring computer reading.
Note: Comparison of non-language computer stimulation vs. no treatment revealed significant between-group differences (PICA: Pantomime score), favouring non-verbal computer stimulation.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that technology-assisted training is not more effective than comparison interventions (nonlinguistic cognitive training) in improving communication skills in patients with chronic stroke.
Note: However, two fair quality RCTs found that technology-assisted training is more effective than a comparison intervention (non-language computer stimulation) and no treatment in improving communication skills in patients with chronic stroke. Differences in findings could be attributed to the differences in interventions, study design/duration and/or outcome measures used.

Comprehenstion
Conflicting
4

Two fair quality RCTs (Katz & Wertz, 1997; Thompson et al., 2010) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on comprehension in patients with chronic stroke.

The first fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1997) randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation, or no treatment. Comprehension was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Comprehension subtest at post-treatment (6 months). No significant between-group difference was found.

The second fair quality RCT (Thompson et al., 2010) randomized patients to receive computer-automated Treatment of Underlying Forms (TUF), clinician-administered TUF or no treatment. Comprehension was measured by the Comprehension Probe (CP: Object relatives, Object clefts, Object wh-questions) and the Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences – Sentence Comprehension Test (NAVS-SCT: Object relatives, Object wh-questions, Subject relatives, Subject wh-questions) at post-treatment (5 weeks). Significant between-group differences were seen (CP – Object relatives; NAVS-SCT: Objects relatives), favoring the computer-automated TUF vs. no treatment. There were no significant differences between computer-automated and clinician-administered TUF.
Note: Differences between clinician-administered TUF and no treatment were not reported.

Conclusion: There is conflicting evidence (Level 4) from two fair quality RCTs regarding the effects of technology-assisted training on comprehension in patients with chronic stroke. While one fair quality RCT found that computer reading is not more effective than comparison interventions (non-language computer stimulation, no treatment), another fair quality RCT found that a computer-automated Treatment of Underlying Forms program is more effective than no treatment in improving comprehension in patients with chronic stroke.
Note: Differences in findings could be attributed to the difference in the interventions, study design/duration and/or outcome measures used.

Naming
Conflicting
4

Two high quality RCTs (Doesborgh et al., 2004b; Palmer et al., 2012) and one fair quality RCT (investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on naming skills in patients with chronic stroke.

The first high quality RCT (Doesborgh et al., 2004b) randomized patients to receive a multicue computer training program or no treatment. Naming skills were measured by the Boston Naming Test at post-treatment (2 months). No significant between group difference was found.

The second high quality RCT (Palmer et al., 2012) randomized patients to receive computer treatment or no treatment; all patients received usual care. Naming skills were measured by the Object and Action Naming Battery at post-treatment (5 months) and follow-up (8 months). A significant between-group difference was found at post-treatment, favoring computer treatment vs. no treatment. Results did not remain significant at follow-up.

The fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1997) randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation, or no treatment. Word finding and semantic retrieval processes were measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Naming scores at post-treatment (6 months). No between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is conflicting evidence (Level 4) regarding the effect of technology-assisted training on naming skills in patients with chronic stroke. While one high quality RCT and one fair quality RCT found that computer training programs were not more effective than comparison interventions (no treatment, non-language computer stimulation), another high quality RCT found that a computer program was more effective than no treatment.
Note: The differences in findings may be attributed to the differences in the interventions, design study/duration (2 vs. 5 months), and/or outcome measures used.

Reading comprehension
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1992) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on reading comprehension in patients with chronic stroke. This fair quality RCT randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation or no treatment. Reading comprehension was measured by the C-CAT (a non-standardized assessment comprising 232 items from the computer reading program) at post-treatment (6 months). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that computer reading is not more effective than comparison interventions (non-language computer stimulation, no treatment) in improving reading comprehension in patients with chronic stroke.

Repetition
Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1997) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on repetition in patients with chronic stroke. This fair quality RCT randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation, or no treatment. Repetition was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Repetition score at post-treatment (6 months). Comparison of computer reading vs. non-language computer stimulation revealed a significant difference, favoring computer reading. Comparison of computer reading vs. no treatment revealed a significant difference, favouring computer reading.
Note: Comparison of non-language computer stimulation vs. no treatment revealed a significant between-group difference, favouring non-verbal computer stimulation.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that computer reading is more effective than comparison interventions (non-language computer stimulation, no treatment) in improving repetition in patients with chronic stroke.

Sentence production
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Thompson et al., 2010) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on comprehension in patients with chronic stroke. This fair quality RCT randomized patients to receive computer-automated Treatment of Underlying Forms (TUF), clinician-administered TUF or no treatment. Comprehension was measured by the Production Probe (PP: Object relatives, Object clefts, Object wh-questions), the Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences – Sentence Production Priming Test (NAVS-SPPT: Object relatives, Object wh-questions, Subject relatives, Subject wh-questions), and the Cinderella narratives (CN: Mean length of utterance, Words per minute, Complex to simple sentence ratio, Noun to verb ratio, Open to closed class ratio, % grammatical sentences, % verbs with correct arguments) at post-treatment (5 weeks). Comparison of computer-automated TUF vs. no treatment revealed significant between-group differences (PP: Object relatives, Object clefts; NAVS-SPPT – Objects relatives), favoring computer-automated TUF. There were no significant differences between computer-automated and clinician-administered TUF.
Note: Differences between clinician-administered TUF and no treatment were not reported.

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that computer-automated Treatment of Underlying Forms is not more effective than no treatment in improving sentence production in patients with chronic stroke.
Note: significant between-group differences were seen in only 2/3 of 7/14 measures

Spontaneous speech
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) and one fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1997) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on spontaneous speech in patients with chronic stroke.

The high quality RCT (Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) randomized patients to receive intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device or nonlinguistic cognitive training for 4 weeks, before crossing over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks. Spontaneous speech was measured by a semi-standardized interview at post-treatment (4 weeks, 8 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). No significant between-group differences were found at any time point.

The fair quality RCT (Katz & Wertz, 1997) randomized patients to receive computer reading, non-language computer stimulation, or no treatment. Spontaneous speech was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Spontaneous speech score at post-treatment (6 months). No significant between-group difference was found.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT and one fair quality RCT that technology-assisted training is not more effective than comparison interventions (nonlinguistic cognitive training, non-language computer stimulation, no treatment) in improving spontaneous speech in patients with chronic stroke.

Verbal communication
Not Effective
1A

Two high quality RCTs (Doesborgh et al., 2004b; Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on verbal communication in patients with chronic stroke.

The first high quality RCT (Doesborgh et al., 2004b) randomized patients to receive a multicue computer training program or no treatment. Verbal communication was measured by the Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday Language Test – Scale A at post-treatment (2 months). No significant between group difference was found.

The second high quality cross-over design RCT (Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) randomized patients to receive intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device or nonlinguistic cognitive training for 4 weeks, before crossing over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks. Verbal communication was measured by the Amsterdam Nijmegen Everyday Language Test at post-treatment (4 weeks, 8 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). No significant between-group difference was found at any time point.

Conclusion: There is strong evidence (Level 1a) from two high quality RCTs that technology-assisted training is not more effective than comparison interventions (no treatment, nonlinguistic cognitive training) in improving verbal communication in patients with chronic stroke.

Verbal fluency
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Nobis-Bosch et al., 2011) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on verbal fluency in patients with chronic stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device or nonlinguistic cognitive training for 4 weeks, before crossing over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks. Verbal fluency was measured by the Word Fluency Test (Food, Animals) at post-treatment (4 weeks, 8 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). No significant between-group difference was found at any time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that intensive language training using the B.A.Bar electronic learning device is not more effective than a comparison intervention (nonlinguistic cognitive training) in improving verbal fluency in patients with chronic stroke.

Phase not specific to one period - Cognitive behavior language therapy

Communication
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Akabogu et al., 2019) investigated the effect of cognitive behavior language therapy (CBLT) on communication in patients with stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients (stage of stroke recovery not specified) to receive CBLT or no treatment. Communication skills were measured by the Porch Index of Communicative Ability at post-treatment (10 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). Significant between-group differences were found at both time points, favoring CBLT vs. no treatment.
Note: In this study, CBLT principles were combined with targeted, conventional speech therapy aimed to reduce aphasia by helping patients to use their remaining language abilities; restore language abilities; and learn alternative communication ways (e.g. gestures, pictures, or use of electronic devices).

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that CBLT is more effective than no treatment in improving communication skills in patients with stroke.

Unhelpful thoughts and beliefs
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Akabogu et al., 2019) investigated the effect of cognitive behavior language therapy (CBLT) on unhelpful thoughts and beliefs in patients with stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients (stage of stroke recovery not specified) to receive CBLT or no treatment. Speech-language unhelpful thoughts and beliefs were measured by Speech-Language Unhelpful Thoughts and Beliefs Scale at post-treatment (10 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). Significant between-group differences were found at both time points, favoring CBLT vs. no treatment.
Note: In this study, CBLT principles were combined with targeted, conventional speech therapy aimed to reduce aphasia by helping patients to use their remaining language abilities; restore language abilities; and learn alternative communication ways (e.g. gestures, pictures, or use of electronic devices).

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that CBLT is more effective than no treatment in reducing unhelpful thoughts and beliefs in patients with stroke.

Phase not specific to one period - Cognitive-linguistic training

Communication (understandability)
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (de Jong-Hagelstein et al., 2011) investigated the effect of cognitive-linguistic training on communication (understandability) in patients with acute/subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive cognitive-linguistic training or communicative treatment (compensatory strategies and use of residual language skills). Communication (understandability) was measured by the Amsterdam-Nijmegen Everyday Language Test Scale A: Understandability (ANELT-A: Final score, Mean improvement, Severity category) at mid-treatment (3 months) and post-treatment (6 months). No significant between-group differences were found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that cognitive-linguistic training is not more effective than a comparison intervention (communicative treatment) in improving communication (understandability) in patients with stroke.

Fluency
Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (de Jong-Hagelstein et al., 2011) investigated the effect of cognitive-linguistic training on fluency in patients with acute/subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive cognitive-linguistic training or communicative treatment (compensatory strategies and use of residual language skills). Fluency was measured by the Letter Fluency Task (LFT), Semantic Association Test (SAT), Semantic Word Fluency (SWF) at mid-treatment (3 months) and post-treatment (6 months). A significant between-group difference was found in one measure at mid-treatment (SWF) and in one measure (LFT) at post-treatment, favoring cognitive-linguistic training vs. communicative treatment.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that cognitive-linguistic training is more effective than a comparison intervention (communicative treatment) in improving fluency in patients with stroke.

Language processing
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (de Jong-Hagelstein et al., 2011) investigated the effect of cognitive-linguistic training on language processing in patients with acute/subacute stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients to receive cognitive-linguistic training or communicative treatment (compensatory strategies and use of residual language skills). Language processing skills were measured by the Psycholinguistic Assessment of Language Processing in Aphasia (PALPA: Semantic association with low imageability words, Nonword repetition, Auditory lexical decision) at mid-treatment (3 months) and post-treatment (6 months). mid-treatment (3 months) and post-treatment (6 months). No significant between-group differences were found at either time point.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that cognitive-linguistic training is not more effective than a comparison intervention (communicative treatment) in improving language processing skills in patients with stroke.

Phase not specific to one period - Narrative aphasia intervention

Discourse abilities
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Whitworth et al., 2014) investigated the effect of narrative aphasia intervention on discourse abilities in patients with stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive Novel Approach to Real-life communication – Narrative Intervention in Aphasia (NARNIA) or usual care. Discourse abilities were measured at (i) word-level by the Pyramid and Palmtrees Test, Kissing and Dancing Test, the Object and Action Naming Battery (Noun retrieval, Verb retrieval), and the Northwestern Assessment of Verbs and Sentences (NAVS – Verb comprehension, Verb naming); (ii) sentence-level by the NAVS (Sentence Comprehension Test, Argument Structure Production Test) and the Sentence Generation Test; and (iii) discourse level by the Curtin University Discourse Protocol – Everyday Discourse (Overall Input: number of utterances; Single Word Level: heavy verbs, light verbs, mental verbs; Sentence Level: two arguments, three arguments, thematic; Discourse: orientation, body, conclusion) at post-treatment (5 weeks). A significant between-group differences was found in only one measure (Everyday Discourse -Discourse: orientation), favoring NARNIA vs. usual care.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that narrative aphasia intervention is not more effective than a comparison intervention (usual care) in improving discourse abilities in patients with subacute/chronic stroke.

Phase not specific to one period - Speech language therapy-unspecified

Abstract reasoning
Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Lincoln et al., 1982) investigated the effect of speech language therapy (SLT) on abstract reasoning in patients with stroke. This fair quality cross-over design RCT randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive (i) SLT or (ii) operant training or attention placebo for 4 weeks; participants then crossed over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks (see group assignment below). Abstract reasoning was measured by the Raven’s Progressive Matrices at post-treatment (4 weeks). A significant between-group difference was found, favoring SLT vs. attention placebo. There were no significant differences between SLT and operant conditioning.
Note: Participants were assigned to 1 of 4 groups – Group A (SLT then attention placebo), Group B (attention placebo then SLT), Group C (SLT then operant conditioning), Group D (operant conditioning then SLT).

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that speech language therapy is more effective than a comparison intervention (attention placebo treatment) in improving abstract reasoning in patients with stroke.
Note: SLT was not more effective than operant conditioning.

Aphasia
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Lincoln et al., 1982) investigated the effect of speech-language therapy (SLT) on aphasia in patients with stroke. This fair quality cross-over design RCT randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive (i) SLT or (ii) operant training or attention placebo for 4 weeks; participants then crossed over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks (see group assignment below). Aphasia was measured by the Token Test – short version at post-treatment (4 weeks). No significant between-group differences were found.
Note: Participants were assigned to 1 of 4 groups – Group A (SLT then attention placebo), Group B (attention placebo then SLT), Group C (SLT then operant conditioning), Group D (operant conditioning then SLT).

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than comparison interventions (attention placebo treatment, operant conditioning) in improving aphasia in patients with stroke.

Communication
Not Effective
2A

Two fair quality RCTs (David et al., 1982; Lincoln et al., 1982) investigated the effect of speech language therapy (SLT) on communication in patients with stroke.

The first fair quality RCT (David et al., 1982) randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive SLT or language stimulation by untrained volunteers. Communication skills were measured by the Functional Communication Profile at different treatment intervals (1, 2, 3, 4 weeks of treatment) and at post-treatment (15-20 weeks). No significant between-group difference was found at any time point.

The second fair quality cross-over design RCT (Lincoln et al., 1982) randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive (i) SLT or (ii) operant training or attention placebo for 4 weeks; participants then crossed over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks (see group assignment below). Communication skills were measured by the Porch Index of Communicative Ability at post-treatment (4 weeks). No significant between-group differences were found.
Note: Participants were assigned to 1 of 4 groups – Group A (SLT then attention placebo), Group B (attention placebo then SLT), Group C (SLT then operant conditioning), Group D (operant conditioning then SLT).

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from two fair quality RCTs that speech language therapy is not more effective than comparison interventions (stimulation and support by untrained volunteers, operant conditioning, non-specific attention placebo treatment) in improving communication skills in patients with stroke.

Fluency
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Lincoln et al., 1982) investigated the effect of speech language therapy (SLT) on fluency in patients with stroke. This fair quality cross-over design RCT randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive (i) SLT or (ii) operant training or attention placebo for 4 weeks; participants then crossed over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks (see group assignment below). Fluency was measured by the Fluency (Food/Countries) and Picture Description tasks at post-treatment (4 weeks). No significant between-group differences were found.
Note: Participants were assigned to 1 of 4 groups – Group A (SLT then attention placebo), Group B (attention placebo then SLT), Group C (SLT then operant conditioning), Group D (operant conditioning then SLT).

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than comparison interventions (attention placebo treatment, operant conditioning) in improving fluency in patients with stroke.

Functional speech
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Lincoln et al., 1982) investigated the effect of speech language therapy (SLT) on functional speech in patients with stroke. This fair quality cross-over design RCT randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive (i) SLT or (ii) operant training or attention placebo for 4 weeks; participants then crossed over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks (see group assignment below). Functional Speech was measured by the Speech Questionnaire at post-treatment (4 weeks). A significant between-group difference in change scores was found, favoring attention placebo vs. SLT. There were no significant differences between SLT and operant conditioning.
Note: Participants were assigned to 1 of 4 groups – Group A (SLT then attention placebo), Group B (attention placebo then SLT), Group C (SLT then operant conditioning), Group D (operant conditioning then SLT).

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (attention placebo treatment, operant conditioning) in improving functional speech in patients with stroke.

Naming
Not Effective
2A

One fair quality RCT (Lincoln et al., 1982) investigated the effect of speech language therapy (SLT) on functional speech in patients with stroke. This fair quality cross-over design RCT randomized patients with subacute/chronic stroke to receive (i) SLT or (ii) operant training or attention placebo for 4 weeks; participants then crossed over to receive the other intervention for a further 4 weeks (see group assignment below). Functional Speech was measured by the Speech Questionnaire at post-treatment (4 weeks). A significant between-group difference in change scores was found, favoring attention placebo vs. SLT. There were no significant differences between SLT and operant conditioning.
Note: Participants were assigned to 1 of 4 groups – Group A (SLT then attention placebo), Group B (attention placebo then SLT), Group C (SLT then operant conditioning), Group D (operant conditioning then SLT).

Conclusion: There is limited evidence (Level 2a) from one fair quality RCT that speech language therapy is not more effective than a comparison intervention (attention placebo treatment, operant conditioning) in improving functional speech in patients with stroke.

Phase not specific to one period - Technology-assisted training

Aphasia
Not Effective
1B

One high quality RCT (Kesav et al., 2017) investigated the effect of technology-assisted training on aphasia in patients with stroke. This high quality RCT randomized patients with acute/subacute stroke to receive computer-based language rehabilitation therapy or no additional treatment; both groups received conventional speech language therapy. Aphasia was measured by the Western Aphasia Battery – Aphasia Quotient at post-treatment (4 weeks) and follow-up (3 months). A significant between-group difference was found at both time points, favoring no additional treatment vs. computer-based language rehabilitation therapy.

Conclusion: There is moderate evidence (Level 1b) from one high quality RCT that technology-assisted training is not more effective than a comparison intervention (conventional speech language therapy alone) in improving aphasia in patients with stroke.
Note: The high quality RCT found that computer-based language rehabilitation therapy + conventional speech language therapy was less effective than speech language therapy alone.

References

Reference list:

  • Akabogu, J., Nnamani, A., Otu, M. S., Ukoha, E., Uloh-Bethels, A. C., Obiezu, M. N., … & Dike, A. E. (2019). Efficacy of cognitive behavior language therapy for aphasia following stroke: Implications for language education research. Medicine, 98(18).https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6504271/
  • Altmann, L. J., Hazamy, A. A., Carvajal, P. J., Benjamin, M., Rosenbek, J. C., & Crosson, B. (2014). Delayed stimulus-specific improvements in discourse following anomia treatment using an intentional gesture. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(2), 439-454.https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/article.aspx?articleid=1802601
  • Benjamin, M. L., Towler, S., Garcia, A., Park, H., Sudhyadhom, A., Harnish, S., … & Gonzalez Rothi, L. J. (2014). A behavioral manipulation engages right frontal cortex during aphasia therapy. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 28(6), 545-553.https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1545968313517754
  • Bowen, A., Hesketh, A., Patchick, E., Young, A., Davies, L., Vail, A., … & Ralph, M. A. L. (2012). Effectiveness of enhanced communication therapy in the first four months after stroke for aphasia and dysarthria: a randomised controlled trial. BMJ, 345, e4407.https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0140673617300673
  • Ciccone, N., West, D., Cream, A., Cartwright, J., Rai, T., Granger, A., … & Godecke, E. (2016). Constraint-induced aphasia therapy (CIAT): a randomised controlled trial in very early stroke rehabilitation. Aphasiology, 30(5), 566-584.https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02687038.2015.1071480
  • David, R., Enderby, P., & Bainton, D. (1982). Treatment of acquired aphasia: speech therapists and volunteers compared. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 45(11), 957-961.https://jnnp.bmj.com/content/45/11/957.short
  • De Jong-Hagelstein, M., Van de Sandt-Koenderman, W. M. E., Prins, N. D., Dippel, D. W. J., Koudstaal, P. J., & Visch-Brink, E. G. (2011). Efficacy of early cognitive–linguistic treatment and communicative treatment in aphasia after stroke: a randomised controlled trial (RATS-2). Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 82(4), 399-404.https://jnnp.bmj.com/content/82/4/399.short
  • Doesborgh, S., van de Sandt‐Koenderman, M., Dippel, D., van Harskamp, F., Koudstaal, P., & Visch‐Brink, E. (2004). Cues on request: The efficacy of Multicue, a computer program for wordfinding therapy. Aphasiology, 18(3), 213-222.https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02687030344000580
  • Doesborgh, S. J., van de Sandt-Koenderman, M. W., Dippel, D. W., van Harskamp, F., Koudstaal, P. J., & Visch-Brink, E. G. (2004). Effects of semantic treatment on verbal communication and linguistic processing in aphasia after stroke: a randomized controlled trial. Stroke, 35(1), 141-146.Effects_of_Semantic_Treatment_on_Verbal
  • Drummond, S. S., & Rentschler, G. J. (1981). The efficacy of gestural cueing in dysphasic word-retrieval responses.https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/handle/2027.42/24335
  • Elman, R. J., & Bernstein-Ellis, E. (1999). The efficacy of group communication treatment in adults with chronic aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42(2), 411-419.https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/article.aspx?articleid=1780893
  • Godecke, E., Ciccone, N. A., Granger, A. S., Rai, T., West, D., Cream, A., … & Hankey, G. J. (2014). A comparison of aphasia therapy outcomes before and after a Very Early Rehabilitation programme following stroke. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(2), 149-161.https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/1460-6984.12074
  • Hartman, J., & Landau, W. M. (1987). Comparison of formal language therapy with supportive counseling for aphasia due to acute vascular accident. Archives of Neurology, 44(6), 646-649.https://jamanetwork.com/journals/jamaneurology/article-abstract/586522
  • Kagan, A., Black, S. E., Duchan, J. F., Simmons-Mackie, N., & Square, P. (2001). Training volunteers as conversation partners using Supported Conversation for Adults with Aphasia (SCA): A controlled trial. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(3), 624-638.https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/article.aspx?articleid=1780646&resultclick=1
  • Katz, R. C., & Wertz, R. T. (1992). Computerized hierarchical reading treatment in aphasia. Aphasiology, 6(2), 165-177.https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02687039208248588
  • Katz, R. C., & Wertz, R. T. (1997). The efficacy of computer-provided reading treatment for chronic aphasic adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(3), 493-507.https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/article.aspx?articleid=1781835
  • Kesav, P., Vrinda, S. L., Sukumaran, S., Sarma, P. S., & Sylaja, P. N. (2017). Effectiveness of speech language therapy either alone or with add-on computer-based language therapy software (Malayalam version) for early post stroke aphasia: A feasibility study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 380, 137-141.https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0022510X17304446
  • Kurland, J., Stanek, E. J., Stokes, P., Li, M., & Andrianopoulos, M. (2016). Intensive language action therapy in chronic aphasia: A randomized clinical trial examining guidance by constraint. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 25(4S), S798-S812.https://ajslp.pubs.asha.org/article.aspx?articleid=2594846
  • Laska, A. C., Kahan, T., Hellblom, A., Murray, V., & Von Arbin, M. (2011). A randomized controlled trial on very early speech and language therapy in acute stroke patients with aphasia. Cerebrovascular Diseases Extra, 1(1), 66-74.https://www.karger.com/Article/Abstract/329835
  • Lincoln, N. B., Mulley, G. P., Jones, A. C., McGuirk, E., Lendrem, W., & Mitchell, J. R. A. (1984). Effectiveness of speech therapy for aphasic stroke patients: a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet, 323(8388), 1197-1200.https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0140673684916908
  • Lincoln, N. B., Pickersgill, M. J., Hankey, A. I., & Hilton, C. R. (1982). An evaluation of operant training and speech therapy in the language rehabilitation of moderate aphasics. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 10(2), 162-178.Behavioural and cognitive psychotherapy
  • Lyon, J. G., Cariski, D., Keisler, L., Rosenbek, J., Levine, R., Kumpula, J., … & Blanc, M. (1997). Communication partners: Enhancing participation in life and communication for adults with aphasia in natural settings. Aphasiology, 11(7), 693-708.https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02687039708249416
  • Maher, L. M., Kendall, D., Swearengin, J. A., Rodriguez, A., Leon, S. A., Pingel, K., … & Rothi, L. J. G. (2006). A pilot study of use-dependent learning in the context of constraint induced language therapy. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(6), 843-852.Journal of the international neuropsychological society
  • Mattioli, F., Ambrosi, C., Mascaro, L., Scarpazza, C., Pasquali, P., Frugoni, M., … & Gasparotti, R. (2014). Early aphasia rehabilitation is associated with functional reactivation of the left inferior frontal gyrus: a pilot study. Stroke, 45(2), 545-552.https://www.ahajournals.org/doi/abs/10.1161/strokeaha.113.003192
  • Nenert, R., Allendorfer, J. B., Martin, A. M., Banks, C., Ball, A., Vannest, J., … & Szaflarski, J. P. (2017). Neuroimaging Correlates of Post-Stroke Aphasia Rehabilitation in a Pilot Randomized Trial of Constraint-Induced Aphasia Therapy. Medical Science Monitor: International Medical Journal of Experimental and Clinical Research, 23, 3489.https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5529460/
  • Nobis-Bosch, R., Springer, L., Radermacher, I., & Huber, W. (2011). Supervised home training of dialogue skills in chronic aphasia: A randomized parallel group study. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(4), 1118-1136.https://jslhr.pubs.asha.org/article.aspx?articleid=1781985&resultclick=1
  • Palmer, R., Enderby, P., Cooper, C., Latimer, N., Julious, S., Paterson, G., … & Delaney, A. (2012). Computer therapy compared with usual care for people with long-standing aphasia poststroke: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Stroke, 43(7), 1904-1911.Computer_Therapy_Compared_With_Usual_Car.pdf
  • Stahl, B., Mohr, B., Dreyer, F. R., Lucchese, G., & Pulvermüller, F. (2016). Using language for social interaction: communication mechanisms promote recovery from chronic non-fluent aphasia. Cortex, 85, 90-99.https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0010945216302684
  • Szaflarski, J. P., Ball, A. L., Vannest, J., Dietz, A. R., Allendorfer, J. B., Martin, A. N., … & Lindsell, C. J. (2015). Constraint-induced aphasia therapy for treatment of chronic post-stroke aphasia: a randomized, blinded, controlled pilot trial. Medical Science Monitor: International Medical Journal of Experimental and Clinical Research, 21, 2861.https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4588672/
  • Sickert, A., Anders, L. C., Münte, T. F., & Sailer, M. (2014). Constraint-induced aphasia therapy following sub-acute stroke: a single-blind, randomised clinical trial of a modified therapy schedule. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry, 85(1), 51-55.https://jnnp.bmj.com/content/85/1/51.short
  • Thompson, C. K., Choy, J. J., Holland, A., & Cole, R. (2010). Sentactics®: Computer-automated treatment of underlying forms. Aphasiology, 24(10), 1242-1266.https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02687030903474255
  • Whitworth, A., Leitao, S., Cartwright, J., Webster, J., Hankey, G. J., Zach, J., … & Wolz, V. (2015). NARNIA: A new twist to an old tale. A pilot RCT to evaluate a multilevel approach to improving discourse in aphasia. Aphasiology, 29(11), 1345-1382.https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/02687038.2015.1081143
  • Woldag, H., Voigt, N., Bley, M., & Hummelsheim, H. (2017). Constraint-induced aphasia therapy in the acute stage: What is the key factor for efficacy? A randomized controlled study. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair, 31(1), 72-80.https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/1545968316662707

Excluded Studies:

Bonifazi, S., Tomaiuolo, F., Altoè, G., Ceravolo, M. G., Provinciali, L., & Marangolo, P. (2013). Action observation as a useful approach for enhancing recovery of verb production: new evidence from aphasiaEur. J. Phys. Rehabil. Med, 49, 473-481.
Reason for exclusion: Not a RCT.

Brady, M. C., Kelly, H., Godwin, J., & Enderby, P. (2012). Speech and language therapy for aphasia following strokeCochrane database of systematic reviews, (5).
Reason for exclusion: Review; all references of this review were verified for inclusion/exclusion in the present module.

Cherney, L. R. (2010). Oral reading for language in aphasia (ORLA): Evaluating the efficacy of computer-delivered therapy in chronic nonfluent aphasiaTopics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 17(6), 423-431.
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received therapy; mode of delivery differed (computer vs. therapist).

Crerar, M. A., Ellis, A. W., & Dean, E. C. (1996). Remediation of sentence processing deficits in aphasia using a computer-based microworld. Brain and Language, 52(1), 229-275.
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received therapy, no control group.

Des Roches, C. A., & Kiran, S. (2017). Technology-based rehabilitation to improve communication after acquired brain injury. Frontiers in neuroscience, 11, 382
Reason for exclusion: Review; all references of this review were verified for inclusion/exclusion in the present module.

Dignam, J., Copland, D., McKinnon, E., Burfein, P., O’brien, K., Farrell, A., & Rodriguez, A. D. (2015). Intensive versus distributed aphasia therapy: a nonrandomized, parallel-group, dosage-controlled study. Stroke, 46(8), 2206-2211.
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received therapy, only intensity varied.

Ferguson, N. F., Evans, K., & Raymer, A. M. (2012). A comparison of intention and pantomime gesture treatment for noun retrieval in people with aphasiaAmerican Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 21(2), S126-S139.
Reason for exclusion: Not a RCT.

Hinckley, J. J., Patterson, J. P., & Carr, T. H. (2001). Differential effects of context-and skill-based treatment approaches: Preliminary findings. Aphasiology, 15(5), 463-476.
Reason for exclusion: All groups received therapy; no control group.

Humphreys, I., Thomas, S., Phillips, C., & Lincoln, N. (2015). Cost analysis of the Communication and Low Mood (CALM) randomised trial of behavioural therapy for stroke patients with aphasiaClinical rehabilitation, 29(1), 30-41
Reason for exclusion: Cost-effectiveness report, no outcome of interest.

Godecke, E., Hird, K., Lalor, E. E., Rai, T., & Phillips, M. R. (2012). Very early poststroke aphasia therapy: a pilot randomized controlled efficacy trial. International Journal of Stroke, 7(8), 635-644.
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received therapy, only intensity differed.

Godecke, E., Armstrong, E. A., Rai, T., Middleton, S., Ciccone, N., Whitworth, A., … & Cadilhac, D. A. (2016). A randomized controlled trial of very early rehabilitation in speech after stroke.
Reason for exclusion: Study protocol.

Latimer, N. R., Dixon, S., & Palmer, R. (2013). Cost-utility of self-managed computer therapy for people with aphasiaInternational journal of technology assessment in health care, 29(4), 402-409.
Reason for exclusion: Cost-analysis report, no outcome of interest.

Martins IP, Leal G, Fonseca I, Farrajota L, Aguiar M, Fonseca J, et al. A randomized, rater-blinded, parallel trial of intensive speech therapy in sub-acute post-stroke aphasia: the SP-IR-IT study. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 48(4), 421-31.
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received therapy, only intensity varied.

Rochon, E., Laird, L., Bose, A., & Scofield, J. (2005). Mapping therapy for sentence production impairments in nonfluent aphasiaNeuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15(1), 1-36.
Reason for exclusion: Not a RCT (n=5 patients).

Thomas, S. A., Walker, M. F., Macniven, J. A., Haworth, H., & Lincoln, N. B. (2013). Communication and Low Mood (CALM): a randomized controlled trial of behavioural therapy for stroke patients with aphasiaClinical rehabilitation, 27(5), 398-408.
Reason for exclusion: Study included in the Depression module.

Szelag, E., Lewandowska, M., Wolak, T., Seniow, J., Poniatowska, R., Pöppel, E., & Szymaszek, A. (2014). Training in rapid auditory processing ameliorates auditory comprehension in aphasic patients: a randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of the neurological sciences, 338(1-2), 77-86.
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received computerized training; no control group.

Smith, D. S., Goldenberg, E., Ashburn, A. E., Kinsella, G., Sheikh, K., Brennan, P. J., … & Reeback, J. S. (1981). Remedial therapy after stroke: a randomised controlled trial. Br Med J (Clin Res Ed), 282(6263), 517-520.
Reason for exclusion: No outcome of interest.

Van der Meulen, I., van de Sandt-Koenderman, W. M. E., Heijenbrok-Kal, M. H., Visch-Brink, E. G., & Ribbers, G. M. (2014). The efficacy and timing of melodic intonation therapy in subacute aphasiaNeurorehabilitation and neural repair, 28(6), 536-544.
Reason for exclusion: Study included in the “Music Therapy” module.

Varley, R., Cowell, P. E., Dyson, L., Inglis, L., Roper, A., & Whiteside, S. P. (2016). Self-administered computer therapy for apraxia of speech: two-period randomized control trial with crossover. Stroke, 47(3), 822-828.
Reason for exclusion: The intervention was designed for apraxia of speech and not aphasia.

Wan, C. Y., Zheng, X., Marchina, S., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2014). Intensive therapy induces contralateral white matter changes in chronic stroke patients with Broca’s aphasiaBrain and language, 136, 1-7.
Reason for exclusion: Study included in the “Music Therapy” module.

Wenke, R., Lawrie, M., Hobson, T., Comben, W., Romano, M., Ward, E., & Cardell, E. (2014). Feasibility and cost analysis of implementing high intensity aphasia clinics within a sub-acute setting. International journal of speech-language pathology, 16(3), 250-259.
Reason for exclusion: Not a RCT.

Wilssens, I., Vandenborre, D., van Dun, K., Verhoeven, J., Visch-Brink, E., & Mariën, P. (2015). Constraint-induced aphasia therapy versus intensive semantic treatment in fluent aphasiaAmerican Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 24(2), 281-294.
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received therapy.

Whiteside, S. P., Inglis, A. L., Dyson, L., Roper, A., Harbottle, A., Ryder, J., … & Varley, R. A. (2012). Error reduction therapy in reducing struggle and group behaviours in apraxia of speech. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 22(2), 267-294.
Reason for exclusion: The intervention was designed for apraxia of speech and not aphasia.

Woolf, C., Caute, A., Haigh, Z., Galliers, J., Wilson, S., Kessie, A., … & Marshall, J. (2016). A comparison of remote therapy, face to face therapy and an attention control intervention for people with aphasia: a quasi-randomised controlled feasibility study. Clinical Rehabilitation, 30(4), 359-373
Reason for exclusion: Both groups received therapy; mode of delivery was different.

Zhang, J., Yu, J., Bao, Y., Xie, Q., Xu, Y., Zhang, J., & Wang, P. (2017). Constraint-induced aphasia therapy in post-stroke aphasia rehabilitation: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PloS one, 12(8), e0183349.
Reason for exclusion: Review; all references of this review were verified for inclusion/exclusion in the present module.

What do you think?